CHAPTER II
DISCUSSION
A. Group and Phrases
The three functional
components of meaning, ideational, interpersonal and textual, are realized
throughout the grammar of a language. But whereas in the grammar of the clause
each component contributes a more or less complete structure, so that a clause
is made up of three distinct structures combined into one , when we look below
the clause, and consider the grammar of the group, the pattern is somewhat
different. Although we can still recognize the same three components, they are
not represented in the form of separate whole structures, but rather as partial
contributions to a single structural line.
The difference between
clause and group in this respect is only one of degree; but it is sufficient to
enable us to analyse the structure of the group in one operation, rather than
in three operations as we did with the clause. At the same time, in
interpreting group structure we have to split the ideational metafunction into
two modes of construing experience: experiential and logical. So far what we
have been describing under the ideational heading has been meaning as
organization of experience; but there is also a logical aspect to it – language
as the construal of certain very general logical relations – and it is this we
have to introduce now.
It comes in at this
point because a group is in some respects equivalent to a word complex – that
is, a combination of words built up on the basis of a particular logical
relation. This is why it is called a group (‘group of words’). It is also the
reason why in the western grammatical tradition it was not recognized as a
distinct structural unit: instead, simple sentences (that is, clauses, in our
terms) were analysed directly into words. Such an analysis is still feasible
provided we confine our attention to the sort of idealized isolated sentences
that grammarians have usually dealt with, such as Socrates runs or John
threw the ball 1; even there, however, the ‘words-in-sentences’ model
ignores several important aspects of the meanings involved, and in the analysis
of real-life discourse it leads to impossible complexity. Describing a sentence
as a construction of words is rather like describing a house as a construction
of bricks, without recognizing the walls and the rooms as intermediate
structural units.
We shall examine the
structure of the three main classes of group: nominal group, verbal group and
adverbial group; along with a brief reference to preposition and conjunction
groups. These classes of group are complexes of nominals, verbs, and adverbs, for
example:
nominal group: the police
verbal group: will conduct
nominal group: an investigation
adverbial group: thoroughly and efficiently.
They serve different
functions in the clause (unless they are rankshifted and embedded in other
units). In terms of the modal structure of the clause, nominal groups serve as
Subject, Complement or Vocative, verbal groups as Finite + Predicator, and
adverbial groups as Adjunct (either circumstantial or modal ones); and in terms
of the experiential structure, nominal groups serve in participant roles,
verbal groups as Process, and adverbial groups in circumstance roles. The
correspondences between class and function were illustrated
A phrase is different from
a group in that, whereas a group is an expansion of a word, a phrase is a
contraction.
B. Nominal group
Those two splendid old electric trains Deictic Numerative
Epithet, Epithet Classifier Thing determiner numeral adjective adjective
adjective noun.
·
Experiential structure of the nominal group: from Deictic to
Classifier
Categorization within
the class is typically expressed by one or more of the functional
elements Deictic, Numerative, Epithet and Classifier.
They serve to realize terms within
different systems of the system network of the
nominal group. We will consider each of
these systems and elements in turn.
1. Deictic
The Deictic element
indicates whether or not some specific subset of the Thing is intended and if
so, which. The nature of the Deictic is determined by the system of
DETERMINATION: The primary distinction is between (i) specific or (ii)
non-specific. Here is an example of the use of Deictics from the beginning of a
narrative; as we shall note below, the absence of the Deictic element is also
systemically meaningful within the system of non-specific determination.
2. Numerative
The Numerative element
indicates some numerical feature of the particular subset of the Thing: either
quantity or order, either exact or inexact.
a) The quantifying Numeratives (or ‘quantitatives’)
specify either an exact number (cardinal numerals, e.g. two trains) or
an inexact number (e.g. many trains, lots of trains); for
example: the Senate confirmed seven Cabinet secretaries .
b) The ordering Numeratives (or ‘ordinatives’) specify
either an exact place in order (ordinal numerals, e.g. the second train)
or an inexact place (e.g. a subsequent train). For example: For the
third time in a decade, the telephone company is changing people’s phone
numbers.
3. Epithet
The Epithet indicates
some quality of the subset, e.g. old, long, blue, fast;
since qualities are denoted by adjectives. Tucker (1998) provides a detailed, lexicogrammatical
and semantic description of adjectives in English, with system networks showing
the potential for construing qualities. (i) The quality of the subset may be an
objective property of the thing itself, construed as a depiction of the
experience of the entity that it represents; or
(ii) it may be an expression of the speaker’s subjective attitude
towards it, e.g. splendid, silly, fantastic (see Poynton,
1996). We refer to these as (i) experiential Epithets and (ii) interpersonal,
or attitudinal, Epithets, respectively.
4. Classifier
The Classifier
indicates a particular subclass of the thing in question, e.g. electric
trains, passenger trains, toy trains. Sometimes the same word
may function either as Epithet or as Classifier, with a difference in meaning:
e.g. fast trains may mean either ‘trains that go fast’ (fast =
Epithet) or ‘trains classified as expresses’ (fast = Classifier). The
line between Epithet and Classifier is not a very sharp one, but there are
significant differences. Classifiers do not accept degrees of comparison or
intensity – we cannot have a more electric train or a very electric
train; and they tend to be organized in mutually exclusive and exhaustive
sets – a train is either electric, steam or diesel.
The range of semantic relations that may be embodied in a set of items
functioning as Classifier is very broad; it includes material, scale and scope,
purpose and function, status and rank, origin, mode of operation – more or less
any feature that may serve to classify a set of things into a system of smaller
sets; for example:
Prison guards restrain the inmates with electric
shock stun guns, leg irons, pepper spray and restraint chairs.
·
Experiential functions and word classes
Deictic Deictic2 Numerative
Epithet Classifier Thing determiner adjective numeral adjective noun or
adjective noun But
there are other possibilities: for example, numeral occurring as Classifier, as
in first prize, or embedded nominal group as possessive Deictic, e.g. the
day before yesterday’s paper. These word classes – noun (= common noun), adjective,
numeral and determiner – are all different
kinds of noun; they are sub-classes of this one primary class. This larger
class can be referred to as ‘nominals’, to avoid confusion with
‘noun’ in its narrower, more specific sense.
Other words also enter into the nominal group, namely words of the class verb, which
may function as Epithet or Classifier. Verbs function in the nominal group in
one of
two forms:
i.
present (active) participle, V-ing, e.g. losing,
as in a losing battle;
ii.
past (passive, or intransitive) participle, V-en, e.g. lost,
as in a lost cause. When functioning as Epithet, these forms usually
have the sense of the finite tense to which they are most closely related: the
present participle means ‘which is (was/will be) ... ing’, the past participle
means ‘which has (had/will have) been ... ed’. When functioning as Classifier,
they typically have the sense of a simple present, active or passive: present
(= active) ‘which ... s’, past (= passive) ‘which is ... ed’.
·
Experiential structure of the nominal group: interpretation of
ordering;
the Qualifier
Ø Ordering
We can now follow the
experiential pattern that is embodied in nominal group structure. Proceeding
from left to right, we begin with the immediate context, the identification of
the item in terms of the here-&-now, e.g. those trains ‘the trains
you can see over there’. Of course, this identification is often in terms of
the surrounding text rather than the situation, e.g. those trains ‘the
trains you’ve just been talking about’; but the point of reference is still the
speech event. From there we go on to quantitative features: place in order, and
number. These are less naturally definitive than this or that, mine or yours,
but more so than a merely qualitative attribute; and the ordinals, being the
more definitive of the two, come first. An ordinal is a kind of superlative
cardinal: third = ‘three-est’, i.e. identified by being at number three.
Next come the qualitative features, again with superlatives preceding others: the
oldest trains ‘trains for which oldness is the identifying feature’.
Often there is an intensifier, such as very, or an attitudinal element
like nice, terrible as a marker of the quality. Finally, comes
class membership; this reduces the size of the total set referred to in the
noun by specifying a sub-set, e.g. passenger train ‘kind of train that
is for carrying passengers’. We are talking here, it should be made clear, of
the identifying potential of these elements. In any actual instance, the
item in question may or may not be identifying; and this is the function of the
word the at the beginning of the group – to signal that something that
is capable of identifying is actually functioning in this way. So there is a
progression in the nominal group from the kind of element that has the greatest
specifying potential to that which has the least; and this is the principle of
ordering that we have already recognized in the clause.
Ø Qualifier
What of the element
which follows the Thing? The original example, Look at those two splendid
old electric trains with pantographs, ended with the phrase with
pantographs; this also is part of the nominal group, having a
function we shall refer to as Qualifier. Unlike the elements that
precede the Thing, which are words (or sometimes word complexes, like two
hundred, very big; see Section 6.3.2), what follows the Thing is
either a phrase or a clause. For example:
Guinness, who was knighted in 1959, had a long film
partnership [with director David Lean].
·
Experiential structure of the nominal group: the Thing
The element we are
calling Thing is the semantic core of the nominal group. It may be realized by
a common noun, proper noun or (personal) pronoun. The personal pronoun
represents the world according to the speaker, in the context of a speech
exchange. The basic distinction is into speech roles (I, you) and
other roles (he, she, it, they); there is also the generalized
pronoun (one).
Ø A note on interpersonal and textual contributions
We noted at the
beginning of the chapter that in analysing group structure it is not necessary to
set up three distinct ‘lines’ corresponding to the experiential, interpersonal
and textual metafunctions. A single structural representation will suffice. We
have been able to express this in experiential terms, because it is a general
principle of linguistic structure that it is the experiential meaning that most
clearly defines constituents ( Halliday, 1979). Interpersonal meanings tend to
be scattered prosodically throughout the unit; while textual meanings tend to
be realized by the order in which things occur, and especially by placing of
boundaries. These are very general tendencies, worked out differently in every
language but probably discernible in all.
Ø Logical structure of the nominal group
We now need to
consider the structure of the nominal group from a different, and complementary,
point of view; seeing it as a logical structure. This does not mean interpreting
it in terms of formal logic; it means seeing how it represents the generalized logical-semantic
relations that are encoded in natural language. For the purposes of the nominal
group we need to take account
5. Head and Thing
We have assumed so far
that the Head of the univariate structure is also always the Thing, in the
multivariate structure. But this is not so. There is always a Head in the
nominal group (unless it is ‘branched’, like one brown in one blue eye
and one brown [Δ]); but there may be no Thing.
There is one
functional environment in which we regularly find Epithet as Head, namely .The
complex functional entity formed by the conflation of Head and Thing acts as
the fulcrum of the nominal group: before it, as Premodifier, a sequence of
words having distinct experiential functions; after it, as Postmodifier, one or
more embedded items which may be prepositional phrase or non-finite or finite
clause. The Premodifier can then be interpreted in logical terms as a
hypotactic word complex.
C.
Verbal Group
The verbal group is the constituent that functions as Finite plus
Predicator (or as Predicator alone if there is no Finite element) in the mood
structure of the clause (clause as exchange) and as process in the transtivity
structure of the clause (clause as representation). In the clause Someone’s
been eating my porridge. The verbal group is has been eating .
A verbal group is the expansion of a verb, in the same way that a
nominal group is the expansion of a noun, and it consists of a sequence of
words of the primary class of verb.
1.
Experiental
structure of the verbal group
The experiental structure of the finite verbal group is Finite
(standing for ‘Finite operator’) plus Event, with optional elements Auxiliary
(one more) and polarity. Finite verbal group range from short, one word items.
ate
|
Finite/Event
|
Couldn’t
|
Have
|
Been
|
Going to
|
Be
|
Being
|
eaten
|
Finite
|
Auxiliary1
|
Auxiliary2
|
Auxiliary3
|
Auxiliary4
|
Auxiliary5
|
Event
|
A striking feature of this structure is its parallelism with the
nominal group. The verbal group begins with the Finite, which is the verbal
equivalent of the Deictic, relating the process to the ‘speaker now’ the Finite
does so by tense or modality. The verbal group ends with the Event, which is
the verbal equivalent of the thing, the former expresses an entity of some
kind, but both represent the core of the lexical meaning.
Both verbal and nominal group begin with the element that fixes the
group in relation to the speech exchange, and both end with the element that
specifies the representational content the difference being that, since things
are more highly organized than events, there are additional lexical elements in
the nominal but none in the verbal group. And it is not difficult to explain
why the structure should be this way round. Initial position is thematic, and
the natural theme of a process or participant is its relation to the here and
now. Final position is informative, and the newsworthy component of a process
or participant is some aspect of its lexical content. So the structure of
groups recapitulates, in the fixed ordering of their elements, the meaning that
is incorporated as choice in the message structure of the clause.
2.
Logical
structure of the verbal group
The verbal group is also structured logically, but in a way that is
quite different from, and has no parallel in the nominal group. The logical
structure of the verbal group realizes the system of tense.
Primary
|
Secondary
|
|
past
|
V-ed (simple past tense)
as in was/were, took, walked
|
Have+V-en
as in have been, have taken, have walked
|
present
|
V-s (simple present tense)
As in is/are, takes, walks
|
Be+V-ing
As in be being, be taking, be walking
|
future
|
Will+V(infinitive)
As in will be, will take, will walk
|
Be going to + V (infinitive)
As in be going to be, be going to take,
be going to walk
|
In naming the tense, it is best to work backwards, beginning with
the deepest and using the preposition in to express the serial modification.
Clearly it is possible to represent every instance of a verbal group by a
structural analysis showing the auxiliaries, in a way that is parallel to what
is done for the nominal group.
3.
Finite,
sequent and non-finite tense systems
There are, in fact, three distinct systems of tense in English:
System I: finite 36
tenses
System II:
sequent 24 tenses
System III: non-finite/modalized 12 tenses
The finite system, system I, is the
one displayed in the centre columns of Table 6-13. The way it works can be
illustrated by building up clauses with associated time expressions. It will be
noted that the order of time Adjuncts is the reverse of that of the tenses,
there is what is known as ‘mirror concord’ between them, invariable except that
the one corresponding to the primary tense can be picked out and made thematic,
e.g. by now she’s known for some time, for a while she was going to have known
already be tonight. The clause chosen is one of mental process, so as to be
able to be built up naturally from the simple present.
System II is that which is available
following a past projection. What happens here is that in the environment of a
‘past’ feature, the past element ih three of the system II tenses is
neutralized, past, ost in present and past in past all represented as past in
past.
System III is the tense system
available in non-finite and in modalized forms of the verbal group. Derivation
of system III by the neutralization of certain contrast in system I shows the
combined effect of both these steps.
4.
Feature
of serial tense
It would be possible, obviously, to
think of all the tense in any one of these systems-finite, sequent or
non-finite as making up a simple set. But this would leave unaccounted for both
the relations among the three systems and the serial character of the tense
system as a whole.
What is remarkable about serial
tense is its regularity, the way in which each choice of tense, whether past,
present or future, defines a location in time which is then used as the point
of departure for a further choice among the same three tenses. This regularity
is obscured, and distorted, by the categories of the structuralist analysis,
and especially the ‘aspect’nomenclature of perfect and progressive (or
continuous).
5.
The
system network of the verbal group
As we have seen, the verbal group is
highly grammaticalized, all elements of its structure except for the event are
realized by grammatical items, the event being the only one that is realized by
a lexical item. It follows that the system network of the verbal group is a network
of systems representing contrast that are purely grammatical in nature. The
only systems that extends in delicacy towards distinctions that are realized
lexcillay is the key system of EVENT TYPE the verbal group analogue of the
thing type system of the nominal group. This systems is concerned with
distinctions among verbs relating to their temporal properties (thus
complementing the clausal system of process type, which is concerned with
distinctions among process relating to configurations of process plus
participants).
The systems of the verbal group
derive from different metafunctions:
i.
Textual:
voice, contrast, and ellipsis
ii.
Interpersonal:
polarity, finiteness, and modality
iii.
Experiental:
aspect and event type
iv.
Logical:
secondary tense
The logical system of secondary tense privides the key organization
of the verbal group. This is the system that defines the logical structure
discussed above. The system of voice also contributes an auxiliary to this
structure (be … -en), which functions like a final secondary tense right before
the event.
6.
Phrasal
verbs
The class of word functioning as event in the verbal group
structure is the verb. Phrasal verbs are lexical verbs that consist of more
than just the verb word itself (also discussed under a number of other
headings, including ‘verb particle construcyion’, or ‘VPC’, and ‘multi-word
verb’, or ‘MWV’). they are of two kinds, plus a third, which is a combination
of the other two:
i.
Verb
+ adverb, e.g. look out ‘unearth, retrieve’
ii.
Verb
+ preposition, e.g. look for ‘seek’
iii.
Verb
+ adverb + preposition, e.g. look out for ‘watch for the presence of’
Examples:
i.
Could
you look out for a good recipe for me?
-yes
I’ll look one out in a moment.
ii.
I’m
looking for a needle, could you help me find one?
-Yes
I’ll look for one in a moment.
iii.
Look
out for snakes, there are lots around here.
-Yes
I’ll look out for them.
Expressions of this kind are lexical items, look out, look for, and
look out for belong as separate entries in a thesaurus or dictionary. They are
thus tending more and more to function as grammatical constituents, but this
tendency is far from complete, and grammatically they are rather unstable.
Experientally, a phrasal verb is a single process, rather than process plus
circumstantial element.
D.
Adverbial group, conjunction group, preposition group
1.
Adverbial
group
The adverbial group serves as Adjunct in the modal structure of the
clause, either circumstantial Adjunct or modal Adjunct (mood or comment).
Examples:
a)
Circumstantial
Adjunct
But
you mustn’t take it personally. (UTS/Macquirie Corpus)
Yeah
but it didn’t aggressively market them. (UTS/Macquirie Corpus)
You’ve
coped beautifully tonight, you’ve coped so well compared to how I would have
coped. (UTS/Macquirie Corpus)
b)
Interpersonal
Adjunct, mood
I
actually didn’t have a lot of chicken, I had probably more vegetables.
(UTS/Macquirie Corpus)
c)
Interpersonal
Adjunct, comment
Apparently
he’s got a wife and a couple of kids. (UTS/Macquirie Corpus)
And
I nearly I nearly smashed him in the face frankly. (UTS/Macquirie Corpus)
Otie,
stop licking plates. There’s one person in this room who just loves that
lasagna. Unfortunately, he isn’t human. (UTS/Macquririe Corpus)
Specifically
what have you been working on this evening Bruno? (UTS/Macquirire Corpus)
The adverbial group has an adverb as Head, which may or may not be
accompanied by modifying elements. Adverbial groups serving as circumstantial
Adjunct have an adverb denoting a circumstance as Head.
2.
Conjunction
group
Within the primary word class of
adverbials, there is another class besides adverbs, namely conjunctions. Their
roles in the grammar are described in chapter 7, they form three sub-classes,
namely linker, binder and continuative.
Conjunctions also form word groups
by modification, for example even, if, just, as, not, until, if only. These can
be represented in the same way. Note however that many conjunctive expressions
have evolved from more complex structure, e.g. as soon as, in case, by the
time, nevertheless, in so far as. These can be treated as single elements
without further analysis. They are themselves, of course, subject to
modification, e.g. just in case, almost as soon as.
3.
Preposition
group
Prepositions are not a sub-class of
adverbials functionally they are realted to verbs. But they form groups by
modification, in the same way as conjunctions, e.g. right behind, not without,
way off as in right behind the door, not without some misgivings, all along the
beach, way off the mark.
Again, there are more complex forms,
such as in front of, for the sake of, which can be left unanalyzed. These are
also subject to modification, as in just for the sake of, immediately in front
of. It is important to make a distinction between a preposition group, such as
right behind or immediately in front of, which is a Modifier-Head structure
expanded from and functionally equivalent to a preposition, and a prepositional
phrase, which is not an expansion of anything but a clause like structure in
which the process/predicator function is performed by a preposition and not by
a verb.
Complex prepositions such as in
front (of), for the shake (of), have evolved from prepositional phrase with
front, sake as ‘complement’. Many expressions are determinate between the two,
for example by the side of, as an alternative to, on the grounds of,
expressions like these are on the way to becoming prepositions typically occur
without a Deictic preceding the noun (in front of, not in the front of), and
the noun occurs in the singular only (in front of, not in front of).
E. Prepositional phrase
The prepositional
phrase serves as Adjunct in the modal structure of the clause. Like the
adverbial group, it can serve as circumstantial Adjunct or, less commonly, as
interpersonal Adjunct; and like the conjunction group, it can serve as conjunctive
Adjunct. In addition, it can be rankshifted to serve as Postmodifier in a nominal
group or an adverbial group. Examples:
Ranking, circumstantial Adjunct
Stop for lunch and a swim at Kuta.
Yeah, we were doing that in Adelaide too.
F. Word classes and group
functions
At the beginning of
this chapter, we presented a table showing the relationship between group and
phrase classes and clause functions . As the table shows, the mapping between
classes at group/phrase rank and functions at clause rank is fairly complex: a group/phrase
of a given class can typically serve a number of different clause functions (the
exception being the verbal group). When we move down one step along the rank scale
to consider the relationship between word classes and group/phrase functions,
we find that there is a stronger tendency towards a one-to-one relationship: a
word of a particular class tends to serve only one group/phrase function. The
major exception is the class of adverb; but this is partly a matter of delicacy:
certain adverbs function only as Head, whereas others function only as Modifier
or Sub-Modifier.
Tidak ada komentar:
Posting Komentar